A new story is floating around town: That the panic that ensued after Orson Welles read his infamous play on air was a myth--one that was fabricated by the newspapers and has been building forming into a bigger and better story ever since.
In Slate Magazine Jefferson Pooley
and Michael Socolow write:
“Upwards of a million people, [were] convinced, if only briefly,
that the United States was being laid waste by alien invaders,” narrator Oliver
Platt informs us in the new PBS
documentary commemorating the program. The panic inspired by Welles
made War of the Worlds perhaps
the most notorious event in American broadcast history.
That’s the story you already know—it’s the narrative widely
reprinted in academic textbooks and popular histories. With actors dramatizing
the reaction of frightened audience members (based on contemporaneous letters),
the new documentary, part of PBS’s American
Experience series, reinforces the notion that naïve Americans were
terrorized by their radios back in 1938. So did this weekend’s
episode of NPR’s Radiolab,
which opened with the assertion that on Oct. 30, 1938, “The United States
experienced a kind of mass hysteria that we’ve never seen before.”
There’s only one problem: The supposed panic was so tiny as to
be practically immeasurable on the night of the broadcast. Despite repeated
assertions to the contrary in the PBS and NPR programs, almost nobody was
fooled by Welles’ broadcast.
How does Socolow and Pooley claim that this myth began? They
blame the newspaper industry, claiming that it had lost revenue to the growing
radio and its scheme was to blow Welles’s broadcast so far out of proportion
that it would discredit it. Interesting theory indeed! But if their theory is correct then it would
be a worse hoax than the broadcast, would it not? For many credible newspapers
across the country to purposely write that the broadcast had fooled millions
of Americans when it had not, one would think, would eventually become a
newsworthy item itself. Wouldn’t a journalist come out of the shadows at some
point and write about that? This would be as bad as the U.S faking the moon
landing. Well… not quite, but close.
These two writers don’t have any hard facts to back up
their claims. What they say, instead, is that there aren’t enough hard facts for
everyone else to back up their claims.
What they complain about most is Hadley Cantril’s book published
in 1940 called The Invasion From Mars: A Study In the Psychology of Panic. They
claim that Cantril doesn’t have enough facts or the right facts or I’m not sure
what. I happen to own the book so for those of you who are curious, I will
quote a few pages from it (I cut some paragraphs out for length). Believe me, it’s thorough!
Who Listened?
The best direct evidence upon which to base an estimate of the
number of people who listened to this broadcast is obtained from a poll made by
the American Institute of Public Opinion (AIPO) about six weeks after the
broadcast. In a nation-wide sample of several thousand adults, 12 per cent
answered “Yes” to a the question “Did you listen to the Orson Welles broadcast
of the invasion from Mars” the representativeness of the sample used by the
Gallup survey is basted on the characteristics of the “voting public.” It
therefor contains more men fewer young people, and probably fewer southern
Negros than the actual radio listening public. According to the 1930 census
there are 75,000,000 persons of voting age in the country. Twelve per cent of
this number would indicate that 9,000,000 adults heard the broadcast. If we
consider all persons ten years of age, then according to the 1930 census we
shall have 12 percent of 99,000,000 people, or, almost 12,000,000. It is undoubtedly
true that many children even under ten years of age listen to the radio after
eight o’clock on Sunday evening, especially when we remember that for more than
half of the country this broadcast was at least an hour earlier than eight p.m.
In addition to these young listeners, a large number of youngsters must have
been wakened by frightened parents preparing to flee for their lives.
The AIPO figure is over 100 per cent higher than any other known
measure of this audience. However, since the Institute reaches many small
communities and non telephone homes not regularly sampled by radio research
organizations, its result is probably the most accurate. C.E Hooper, Inc., a
commercial research organization making continuous checks on program
popularity, indicatives a listening audience of about 4,000,000 to the Mercury
Theatre broadcast on October 30, 1938. If we pool the AIPO and Hoooper results,
a final estimage of 6,000,000 listeners is conservative. Had the program enjoyed
greater popularity, the panic might have been more widespread.
How Many Were Frightened?
In answer to the AIPO question, “At the time you were listening,
did you think this broadcast was a play or a news report? 28 per cent indicated
that they believed the broadcast was a news bulletin. Seventy per cent of those
who thought they were listening to a news report were frightened or disturbed.
This would mean that about 1,700,000 heard the broadcast as a news bulletin and
that about 1,200,000 were excited by it.
In spite of the attempt to word the question of concerning
individual’s reaction in a casual way, it must be remembered that the number of
persons who admitted their fright to the AIPO interviewers is probably the very
minimum of the total number actually frightened. Many persons were probably too
ashamed of their gullibility to confess it in a cursory interview.
Sectional differences in extent of fright
New England 40
per cent
Mid Atlantic 69
East north Central 72
West North Central 72
South 80
Mountain and Pacific 71
Telephone volume. Accounts of frantic telephone calls flooding switchboards
of radio stations, news papers and police stations are confirmed by figures
secured from the American Telephone Company. An increase of 39 per cent was
reported in the telephone volume in metropolitan northern New Jersey during the
hour of the broadcast as compared to the usual volume of that hour of the
evening. A 25 percent increase over normal in the same area occurred the
succeeding hour. Increases for several suburban exchanges on Long Island for
the same hours ranged from 5 to 19 per cent. In six suburban exchanges
surrounding Philadelphia, traffic increased 9.6 per cent for the entire day of
October 30 and for the entire New York metropolitan area traffic was above
normal. Telephone officials see no other way to account for the increased
volume than assigning responsibility to the broadcast. The managers of 92 radio
stations that carried the broadcast were questioned by mail about their
telephone volume during and immediately following the program. Of the 52 who replied,
50 reported an increase. Thirty-seven per cent noted increases of at least 500
per cent over the usual Sunday nigh volume.
An analysis of 12,500 newspaper clippings appearing in papers throughout
the country during the three weeks following the broadcast, reveals that,
although he volume of press notices took the usual sharp decline the second and
third days, considerable interest was maintained for five days and had not
fallen below 30 percent of the original volume by the end of the first week. (a
graph is included).
So I have this to say:
1) why do they question Cantril’s book and statistics?
2) Do they have a book with different statistics?
3) Do they have any men who worked for the newspapers who can verify
that they did indeed exaggerate or lie to make radio look bad?
Nonfiction is
always difficult because there are always gray areas. The further in time you
go the more difficult things become. No one is alive anymore to tell you what
really happened. But if you don’t have any concrete facts to question thousands
of newspaper reports and a whole scholarly case study done at the time then I
don’t think you should say anything at all. You should especially not go on NPR
saying that you are positive that everyone
else is wrong. I mean heck… maybe these guys are right… but how can they be so
sure?
No comments:
Post a Comment